2021 Replicability Report for the Psychology Department at Yale

Since 2011, it is an open secret that many published results in psychology journals do not replicate. The replicability of published results is particularly low in social psychology (Open Science Collaboration, 2015).

A key reason for low replicability is that researchers are rewarded for publishing as many articles as possible without concerns about the replicability of the published findings. This incentive structure is maintained by journal editors, review panels of granting agencies, and hiring and promotion committees at universities.

To change the incentive structure, I developed the Replicability Index, a blog that critically examined the replicability, credibility, and integrity of psychological science. In 2016, I created the first replicability rankings of psychology departments (Schimmack, 2016). Based on scientific criticisms of these methods, I have improved the selection process of articles to be used in departmental reviews.

1. I am using Web of Science to obtain lists of published articles from individual authors (Schimmack, 2022). This method minimizes the chance that articles that do not belong to an author are included in a replicability analysis. It also allows me to classify researchers into areas based on the frequency of publications in specialized journals. Currently, I cannot evaluate neuroscience research. So, the rankings are limited to cognitive, social, developmental, clinical, and applied psychologists.

2. I am using department’s websites to identify researchers that belong to the psychology department. This eliminates articles that are from other departments.

3. I am only using tenured, active professors. This eliminates emeritus professors from the evaluation of departments. I am not including assistant professors because the published results might negatively impact their chances to get tenure. Another reason is that they often do not have enough publications at their current university to produce meaningful results.

Like all empirical research, the present results rely on a number of assumptions and have some limitations. The main limitations are that
(a) only results that were found in an automatic search are included
(b) only results published in 120 journals are included (see list of journals)
(c) published significant results (p < .05) may not be a representative sample of all significant results
(d) point estimates are imprecise and can vary based on sampling error alone.

These limitations do not invalidate the results. Large difference in replicability estimates are likely to predict real differences in success rates of actual replication studies (Schimmack, 2022).

Yale

I used the department website to find core members of the psychology department. I counted 13 professors and 4 associate professors, which makes it one of the smaller departments in North America. Not all researchers conduct quantitative research and report test statistics in their result sections. I limited the analysis to 12 professors and 1 associate professors who had at least 100 significant test statistics.

Figure 1 shows the z-curve for all 13,147 tests statistics in articles published by these 18 faculty members. I use the Figure to explain how a z-curve analysis provides information about replicability and other useful meta-statistics.

1. All test-statistics are converted into absolute z-scores as a common metric of the strength of evidence (effect size over sampling error) against the null-hypothesis (typically H0 = no effect). A z-curve plot is a histogram of absolute z-scores in the range from 0 to 6. The 1,178 z-scores greater than 6 are not shown because z-scores of this magnitude are extremely unlikely to occur when the null-hypothesis is true (particle physics uses z > 5 for significance). Although they are not shown, they are included in the meta-statistics.

2. Visual inspection of the histogram shows a steep drop in frequencies at z = 1.96 (solid red line) that corresponds to the standard criterion for statistical significance, p = .05 (two-tailed). This shows that published results are selected for significance. The dashed red line shows significance for p < .10, which is often used for marginal significance. Thus, there are more results that are presented as significant than the .05 criterion suggests.

3. To quantify the amount of selection bias, z-curve fits a statistical model to the distribution of statistically significant results (z > 1.96). The grey curve shows the predicted values for the observed significant results and the unobserved non-significant results. The statistically significant results (including z > 6) make up 31% of the total area under the grey curve. This is called the expected discovery rate because the results provide an estimate of the percentage of significant results that researchers actually obtain in their statistical analyses. In comparison, the percentage of significant results (including z > 6) includes 69% of the published results. This percentage is called the observed discovery rate, which is the rate of significant results in published journal articles. The difference between a 69% ODR and a 31% EDR provides an estimate of the extent of selection for significance. The difference of~ 40 percentage points is fairly large. The upper level of the 95% confidence interval for the EDR is 42%. Thus, the discrepancy is not just random. To put this result in context, it is possible to compare it to the average for 120 psychology journals in 2010 (Schimmack, 2022). The ODR (69% vs. 72%) and the EDR (31% vs. 28%) are similar. This suggest that the research produced by Yale faculty members is neither more nor less replicable than research produced at other universities.

4. The EDR can be used to estimate the risk that published results are false positives (i.e., a statistically significant result when H0 is true), using Soric’s (1989) formula for the maximum false discovery rate. An EDR of 31% implies that no more than 12% of the significant results are false positives, but the lower limit of the 95%CI of the EDR, 18%, allows for 24% false positive results. Most readers are likely to agree that this is too high. One solution to this problem is to lower the conventional criterion for statistical significance (Benjamin et al., 2017). Figure 2 shows that alpha = .005 reduces the point estimate of the FDR to 3% with an upper limit of the 95% confidence interval of 5%. Thus, without any further information readers could use this criterion to interpret results published in articles by researchers in the psychology department of Harvard University.

Given the small size of the department, it is not very meaningful to conduct separate analyses by area. However, I did conduct a z-curve analysis of articles published since 2016 to examine whether research at Yale has changed in response to the call for improvements in research practices. The results show an increase in the expected discovery rate from 31% to 40%, although the confidence intervals still overlap. Thus, it is not possible to conclude at this moment that this is a real improvement (i.e., it could just be sampling error). The expected replication rate also increased slightly from 65% to 70%. Thus, there are some positive trends, but there is still evidence of selection for significance (ODR 71% vs. EDR = 40%).

There is considerable variability across individual researchers, although confidence intervals are often wide due to the smaller number of test statistics. The table below shows the meta-statistics of all 13 faculty members that provided results for the departmental z-curve. You can see the z-curve for individual faculty member by clicking on their name.

Rank   Name ARP EDR ERR FDR
1Tyrone D. Cannon6973643
2Frank C. Keil6777564
3Yarrow Dunham53713510
4Woo-Kyoung Ahn52733112
5B. J. Casey5264398
6Nicholas B. Turk-Browne50663410
7Jutta Joorman49643510
8Brian J. Scholl46692318
9Laurie R. Santos42661725
10Melissa J. Ferguson38621434
11Jennifer A. Richeson37492615
12Peter Salovey36571530
13John A. Bargh35561531

Leave a Reply